How is culture adaptive




















For example, men and women have complementary roles in many cultures. Biology versus Culture : These two avatars illustrate the basic concept of culture. One is simply a reflection of his biology; he is human. The other is a reflection of his biology and his culture: he is human and belongs to a cultural group or sub-culture. Cultural change can have many causes, including the environment, technological inventions, and contact with other cultures.

Cultures are externally affected via contact between societies, which may also produce—or inhibit—social shifts and changes in cultural practices. War or competition over resources may impact technological development or social dynamics.

Additionally, cultural ideas may transfer from one society to another, through diffusion or acculturation. Discovery and invention are mechanisms of social and cultural change. Discovery refers to the finding of new knowledge within an existing realm. Generally, it relates to discovering new understanding of a particular behavior or ritual. Invention is the creation of a new device or process. New discoveries often lead to new inventions by people.

The Change of Symbolic Meaning Over Time : The symbol of the ankh has its roots in Egyptian religious practice, but the symbol diffused over time and was adopted by other groups, including pagans, as a religious symbol.

In diffusion, the form of something though not necessarily its meaning moves from one culture to another. For example, hamburgers, mundane in the United States, seemed exotic when introduced into China. Acculturation has different meanings, but in this context it refers to replacement of the traits of one culture with those of another, such has happened to certain Native American tribes and to many indigenous peoples across the globe during the process of colonization.

Related processes on an individual level include assimilation adoption of a different culture by an individual and transculturation. Produce an example of cultural lag using an example of the tension between material and non-material culture. The term cultural lag refers to the notion that culture takes time to catch up with technological innovations, and that social problems and conflicts are caused by this lag. Cultural lag is not only a concept, as it also relates to a theory and explanation in sociology.

Cultural lag helps to identify and explain social problems and to predict future problems. The term was coined by the sociologist William F.

His theory of cultural lag suggests that a period of maladjustment occurs when the non-material culture is struggling to adapt to new material conditions. This essay will briefly expound on each positive outcome and its counterpart, and will relate the sustainable agrarian achievements of the people of the Gamo Highlands to these effects.

The first positive consequence of agriculture is the stabilization it has allowed humanity. For many years, societies were based around the game its members hunted for food. For example, Native Americans once relied upon buffalo as a main source of meat.

As buffalo do not stay in one location, the Native American people were forced to follow the buffalo, migrating with them wherever the beasts went, in order to maintain this valuable food source. They originated from the area around Wisconsin. There, they fished, hunted, farmed and gathered like any other Eastern Woodlands tribe. Later on, they moved to the Great Plains, where they had the buffalo hunt and the sun dance. Geography affected Cheyenne culture in this way.

The buffalo hunt was created so, since the Great Plains lacked much food, the Cheyenne could get enough food to survive. Native Americans and colonists had different views on the use of land resources.

The Natives viewed the land as something not owned, but as a resource to sustain life. They believe in a hunting-gathering system, hunting only when necessary. Because wolves regulate the carrying capacity, preserve the health of herds, and complete the ecological cycle in a balanced system, they must be restored to Yellowstone. This statement may sound simplified, but actual wolf extermination was thoughtless and thorough. In the old days, the Indians hunted buffalo and used their hide for shelter, their fur for clothing, and their meat for food.

Hunting is also considered as a sport. It also provides students with an extra-curricular activity. Lots of people hunt for entertainment and fun. An evolutionary rescue scenario where the ancestral variant causes excess mortality and the novel variant provides a survival benefit. Figure 5 A shows the interplay between the survival benefit q and the cultural transmission advantage f of the innovation a on the mean time to population rescue, i.

Parameter constellations shown in white do not result in a rescue. We observe a non-linear relationship between q and f : small values of q can be compensated for by large values of f and vice versa leading to a rapid rescue. Figure 5 A shows four parameter domains. The domain labelled i contains the cases where cultural transmission bias makes a rescue possible by speeding up the spread of a weakly beneficial trait, where otherwise the population would collapse.

The domain labelled iv contains the values of q for which an evolutionary rescue is possible even in the absence of a transmission advantage to a. Finally, domains ii and iii contain cases where rescues are always possible or never possible and extinction is likely. Figure 5 B shows the corresponding population bottlenecks, i. As expected, parameter constellations leading to a long rescue time also lead to small bottlenecks. A The mean time to evolutionary rescue and B the severity of the population bottleneck.

Simulations ran until the population was extinct or exceeded its original size, indicating a cultural evolutionary rescue in this parameter range. We can show that where the mismatch between the environment and the ancestral variant A is more pronounced, a rescue is less likely and where they do occur, the population bottleneck is more severe, as one might expect from previous work on evolutionary rescues 11 , 18 , Finally, we consider the cases in which population extinctions do occur despite the introduction of a beneficial cultural variant.

Here, we can show that a weakly beneficial variant, may, sometimes significantly, lengthen the time between the environmental change and population collapse see Fig. In the presence of a beneficial cultural variant, this time to extinction can be increased considerably.

The increase depends on the probability of spread of the cultural variant as well as the protection against death, quantified by q , that it confers compared to variant A. This lends credence to the idea that the spread of a weakly beneficial cultural trait may facilitate true evolutionary rescue for human populations by prolonging the time a population can wait for a beneficial genetic mutation to arise and spread.

The time to population collapse, where cultural rescues do not occur, for different values of q. Threshold for extinction here is 5 individuals and is marked with a red solid line. Black and grey lines show total population sizes, red dashed lines show the frequency of the beneficial cultural trait.

Higher frequencies of the beneficial trait delay population collapse—sometimes substantially. In summary, culture alone may be able to rescue a population, prevent or mitigate population bottlenecks, or extend the survival time of a declining population during which true evolutionary rescue may be possible in a way rare or unique for large, long-living organisms. The uniquely well-developed cognitive and cultural abilities of humans have undoubtedly contributed to our success as a species.

However, we understand little about the ways in which our ability to generate and maintain culture affect our adaptation to new environments and our probability of success in those environments. We describe a model of the cultural adaptive process, examining aspects of adaptation that are unique to culture, and discuss the possible effects of cultural adaptation on population dynamics.

We begin with a simple system that includes a single finite population in which generations overlap—a condition necessary for understanding the effect of various transmission mechanisms. We focus on a single cultural trait with two possible variants: A , an ancestral variant and a , a new innovation. After the shift, however, variant a provides a higher benefit.

In contrast to the genetic case, for a cultural system it is sensible to assume that innovations are plentiful. Therefore, we assume that there is always one new innovation present in the population prior to an environmental shift, i. Under these conditions, we show that if the benefit of variant a , the variant under selection, is the same in both cases, a selective sweep is more likely from standing variation than from a novel innovation arising after the environmental shift.

The frequency dynamic of a before the shift, i. Here we focus on three transmission mechanisms: unbiased transmission, conformist transmission and anti-conformist transmission. We show that the frequency at which variant a is likely to be represented at the time of the environmental shift will change, perhaps dramatically, from one transmission mechanism to another. These changes mean that in the mechanisms we tested the probability of a sweep from standing variation is most likely in the case of anti-conformist transmission where variants are more often represented at intermediate frequencies in the period prior to an environmental change.

This suggests that the dominant transmission mechanisms on which a population relies could have a significant effect on its ability to build and maintain cultural diversity and to use that diversity in the case of environmental shock This echoes the findings of Ref. Above, as in a genetic case, innovations are generally assumed to be blind to the exact nature of the environmental shift. Of course, cultural innovations may not be so blind.

Humans possess a number of sophisticated cognitive abilities that enable us to generate adaptive culture. We consider two. First, traits may be innovated and deliberately maintained in a population even when the trait is not under selection and the benefit is not immediately clear because we consider it likely to be useful in the future.

This ability for foresight means that after an environmental shift a population may possess, in its standing variation, a trait that has higher benefit than we would expect otherwise. We posit that foresight is unlikely to produce a variant that is more beneficial in the novel circumstances than directed innovation.

The model shows that where foresight is poor and directed innovations are relatively good, novel innovations in response to environmental shifts are more likely to produce a cultural selective sweep. In the case where foresight is relatively good or, alternatively, the environmental shift is predictable to some extent a sweep from standing variation is more likely. This is true even when innovation after the shift can produce a cultural trait with a larger benefit.

In cultural systems where foresight and directed innovation are absent 29 and indeed in genetic systems where the same is true 30 , a rapidly fluctuating environment generally favours an increase in innovation rate. However, when foresight is included, a variety of strategies for maintaining useful standing variation may be favoured instead.

For example, selectively maintaining older information, neutral or even currently maladaptive traits, or switching reliance on transmission mechanisms to maintain more information that may be useful in the future.

Our understanding of the interactions between how culture is maintained by a population, the environment and, for example, population size is incomplete and the subject of some controversy e. Our understanding of the effect of population bottlenecks in a cultural system is even more limited but see Ref.

Here we attempt to scratch the surface of these issues. We show that the survival benefit of the trait itself can lead to a population rescue under unbiased transmission. Further, including the effect of biasing transmission towards the beneficial variant can reduce the time to such a rescue.

Cultural traits can produce a rescue where one otherwise would not occur. Finally, and importantly, a beneficial cultural trait can reduce the severity of a population bottleneck. This raises the possibility that a rapid cultural response to a massive environmental shift might maintain the population at higher numbers for longer, enabling a slower evolutionary rescue where one might not otherwise occur. Our results suggest that a gene-culture co-evolutionary model of evolutionary rescue would be more appropriate and indeed may be necessary to fully understand human adaptation specifically.

The widening of a population bottleneck by behavioural or cultural adaptation might also have an important effect on our ability to detect such culturally buffered selective events in genetic data. In fact, it may be that such events could only be detected in cultural data and, at that, for a very short time. As humans moved across the planet, colonising almost every continent and establishing themselves as a hugely successful species from a demographic point of view, populations inevitably experienced many large-scale environmental changes and shifts—from food scarcity to large average temperature differences—from one location to another.

To understand how human populations successfully navigated, and continue to navigate, these challenges we must understand more than just the process of genetic adaptation. Drawing on models like the ones above and existing theory of cultural evolution, we must generate a much deeper understanding of the mechanics of cultural adaptation and its inevitable interactions with our genes.

Hermisson, J. Soft sweeps: Molecular population genetics of adaptation from standing genetic variation. Genetics , — Prezeworski, M. The signature of positive selection on standing genetic variation. Evolution 59 , — Article Google Scholar. Barrett, R. Adaptation from standing genetic variation. Trends Ecol.

Article PubMed Google Scholar. Aoki, K. Rates of cultural change and patterns of cultural accumulation in stochastic models of social transmission. What do we mean by culture is adaptive and maladaptive with examples? What is the characteristics of culture is maladaptive?

Why is it important to remember your culture? How do you maintain your culture? Previous Article Is it OK to cook turkey day before? Next Article Why space is such an important concept of art How did art change following the invention of linear perspective? Back To Top.



0コメント

  • 1000 / 1000